StudySmarter - The all-in-one study app.
4.8 • +11k Ratings
More than 3 Million Downloads
Free
Dollard et al. (1939) proposed the frustration-aggression hypothesis, influenced by Marxism, psychoanalysis and behaviourism. The frustration-aggression hypothesis states that if we experience frustration due to being prevented from achieving a goal, it will lead to aggression - a cathartic release from frustration.Here is an outline of the stages of the hypothesis:
An attempt to achieve a goal is blocked
Frustration occurs
An aggressive drive is created
Aggressive behaviour is displayed (cathartic)
Thus, the aggression created by the frustration is satisfied.
The aggression cannot always be directed at the source of frustration, as the source may be:
Abstract, such as lack of money.
Too powerful, and you risk punishment by showing aggression towards them; for example, a person may be frustrated by their boss at work, but they cannot direct their anger towards the boss for fear of repercussions.
Unavailable at the time; for example, your teacher gives you a bad grade for an assignment, but you do not notice until she has left the classroom.
Due to these reasons, people may direct their aggression towards something or someone else.
A man may not direct his aggression towards his boss, so he shows aggressive behaviour when he comes home later to his family instead.
Dollard et al. modified the frustration-aggression hypothesis in 1941 to state that aggression was one of several outcomes of frustration. They believed the frustration-aggression hypothesis could explain animal, group, and individuals’ behaviours. The frustration-aggression hypothesis has been used to explain real-world behaviour such as scapegoating. In times of crisis and as levels of frustration amass ( for example, during an economic crisis), frustrated groups may release their aggression against a convenient target, often a minority group.
In 1989, Leonard Berkowitz attempted to combine Dollard et al.’s understanding of environmental occurrences of frustration with more recent understandings of frustration as an internal process. He proposed that frustrations are still aversive events, but frustration produces aggressive inclinations only if it causes negative affect (feeling/emotion) in a person. It is negative affect, not frustration, that causes aggressive tendencies.
Negative affect refers to an internal feeling occurring when you have failed to achieve a goal, avoid danger or are not satisfied with the current state of affairs.
It is important to note that Berkowitz did not state that negative affect produces aggressive behaviour but rather ‘aggressive inclinations’. Thus, negative affect produced by frustration does not automatically lead to aggressive behaviour. Many factors might prevent this; for example, a person may reappraise the situation, there may be strong incentives not to behave aggressively.
Berkowitz’s revised frustration-aggression hypothesis (1989)
Depiction of Berkowits’s revised Frustration-Aggression Hypothesis - StudySmarter Originals.
Dill and Anderson (1995) conducted a study testing the role of justified and unjustified frustration. The participants were university students, split into three groups. The participants received instructions on folding an origami bird and were timed while completing this task alone. It was made clear that instructions would only be given once. Success on the task consisted of quickly and successfully folding the origami after the instructions. The speed of the experimenter explaining the instructions was intentionally fast.
There was an experimenter who gave instructions in each group and a confederate who acted as a participant. The confederate would interrupt the experimenter and ask them to give the instructions slowly.
The three conditions:
Unjustified frustration condition: the experimenter responded, ‘I would like to hurry and get this over with. My boy/girlfriend is coming to pick me up. I don’t want to make them wait.’
Justified frustration condition: the experimenter responded, ‘My supervisor has scheduled someone else in this room very shortly and has pressured me to do this as quickly as I can. I’m afraid I’m not able to slow down.’
Control condition: the experimenter responded, ‘Oh, okay., I didn’t realise that I was going too fast. Let me back up a little and go more slowly,’ and slowed down to a more reasonable pace.
After the participants tried the origami task and were given some questionnaires to complete, the questionnaires were about:
Their level of aggression
An evaluation of the university’s teaching and research assistants
It was stated in the questionnaire that the evaluation would help determine those who deserved rewards and financial assistance and those who deserved disciplinary measures such as verbal reprimands and possible reductions in pay.
The results found that justified frustration produced less hostile aggression than unjustified frustration but more hostile aggression than no frustration at all.
The participants were split into three groups, rumination, distraction, or control. Researchers showed the rumination group a picture of the participant who had criticised them (one of 6 pre-selected photos) and told them to hit a punching bag while thinking of that person. The distraction group also hit punching bags but were told to think about physical fitness. The control group sat quietly for a few minutes.
Afterwards, anger and aggression levels were measured. The results found that participants in the rumination group were most angry, followed by the distraction group, then the control group.
There are methodological flaws in some of the studies. For example, the study by Green (1968) only used male university students, making it difficult to generalise the results to females or populations outside university students. Likewise, the study by Dill and Anderson (1995) only used university students too.
The frustration-aggression hypothesis wielded a strong influence on decades of research, but it was criticised for its theoretical rigidity and over-generalisation. Later research was focused more on refining the hypothesis, such as the work of Berkowitz. Some other criticisms were:
The frustration-aggression hypothesis does not explain how aggressive behaviour might arise in different social environments without provocation or feeling frustrated; however, this could be attributed to deindividuation.
Dollard et al. (1939) proposed the frustration-aggression hypothesis. They stated that if we experience frustration by being blocked to attain a goal, this leads to aggression, a cathartic release from frustration.
The aggression cannot always be directed at the source of frustration, as the source may be abstract, too powerful, or not available at the time. Thus, people may displace their aggression towards something or someone else.
The frustration-aggression hypothesis has been used to explain real-world behaviour such as scapegoating.
In 1989, Berkowitz revised the frustration-aggression hypothesis; he proposed that frustrations are still aversive events. However, frustration produces aggressive inclinations only if it causes negative affect (feeling/emotion) in a person. It is negative affect, not frustration, that causes aggressive tendencies.
The frustration-aggression hypothesis suggests that aggressive behaviour is cathartic, but the evidence doesn’t support this idea. There are individual differences in response to frustration. Some studies that show evidence for the frustration-aggression hypothesis only use male/university students making the results hard to generalise. Much research into the frustration-aggression hypothesis has been conducted in laboratory environments; thus, the results have low ecological validity.
Criticisms of the frustration-aggression hypothesis are its theoretical rigidity and overgeneralisation. Also, the frustration-aggression hypothesis does not explain how aggressive behaviour might arise in different social environments without provocation or feeling frustrated. In addition, aggression can be a learned response and does not always happen due to frustration.
Frustration always precedes aggression, and frustration always leads to aggression.
According to Dollard et al., frustration is the ‘condition which exists when a goal-response suffers interference’ and aggression is ‘an act whose goal-response is injury to an organism (or an organism surrogate).’ Organism surrogate - when the aggression is displaced onto something/someone else, not the source of frustration.
The original frustration-aggression hypothesis proposed that if we experience frustration by being blocked to attain a goal, this leads to aggression. Berkowitz revised the hypothesis in 1989 to state that frustration produces aggressive inclinations only if it causes negative affect (feeling/emotion) in a person.
of the users don't pass the Frustration Aggression Hypothesis quiz! Will you pass the quiz?
Start QuizBe perfectly prepared on time with an individual plan.
Test your knowledge with gamified quizzes.
Create and find flashcards in record time.
Create beautiful notes faster than ever before.
Have all your study materials in one place.
Upload unlimited documents and save them online.
Identify your study strength and weaknesses.
Set individual study goals and earn points reaching them.
Stop procrastinating with our study reminders.
Earn points, unlock badges and level up while studying.
Create flashcards in notes completely automatically.
Create the most beautiful study materials using our templates.
Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.